La lettre de la Preuve

       

ISSN 1292-8763

Spring 2002

 

Proof in the curriculum: an overview

 

John Mason
Open University - UK


A meeting recently took place on the role of reasoning and proof in mathematics classrooms, organised by the Qualifications and Curriculum Authority (QCA) for England. This was in some ways a landmark meeting in that a quasi-autonomous non-governmental organisation (QUANGO) which was set up to advise the Minister of State for Education on qualifications and curriculum, chose to invite experts to discuss a topic which the QCA consider to be important but which has not yet been signalled by the Minister as requiring attention. Their aim was to seek advice and information form a range of countries concerning possible directions for revisions to the U.K. National Curriculum. Since the QCA also tests children at the ages of 7, 11 and 14, and provides optional tests for all other years, they do more than just give advice: they actually formulate the National Curriculum and chart learner progress.


So it was that a rather bemused group of people with interests and concerns about teaching mathematical reasoning in schools, met in Cambridge over two days. Fourteen countries were represented: Australia, Bulgaria, Canada, Czechoslovakia, France, Italy, Israel, Japan, Mexico, Poland, Russia, Singapore, UK, USA.


Some colleagues had been asked to describe what was done regarding reasoning and proof in their school curricula. The overwhelming impression was that despite differences of emphasis, of approach and of orientation, success has not been universally great. Where reasoning has been deeply embedded in the ethos of the classroom, learners do appear to be able both to recognise and to construct reasoned justifications for assertions and conjectures. But where concentration has been largely in one domain (perhaps algebra, perhaps geometry) there is relatively little cross-over into the other domain. Where reasoning has been bolted on or assigned to a specific part of the curriculum, a majority of learners failed to demonstrate significant grasp of the idea of mathematical reasoning.


Some colleagues were asked to address general issues concerning reasoning. Emphasis was on psycho-social aspects of epistemological development, and on the construction of tasks which promote mathematical thinking.


A number of questions arose which need to be addressed and for which further research is necessary, including


What needs to be done do prompt children to develop from accepting the authority of outside sources (adults, institutions), to acting responsibly (literal meaning: to be prepared to justify one's actions or assertions) by taking on the mantle of authority for themselves through being able to justify their assertions-claims-conjectures; to develop from "just because!" and "it just is" as warrants, to reasoned justifications?

How is it that children can spontaneously use counter-examples in social discourse, but rarely do in mathematics unless specifically encouraged?

How can children be encouraged to make use of writing and software as a means of expression through which they refine their explanations and justifications; moving from convincing themselves, to convincing a friend who is present so that alterations and further justifications can be offered as needed, to convincing someone who is not present and so not relying on responding to their questions and concerns but rather envisaging what these might be and addressing them?

How can an ethos of scientific-debate, of mathematical thinking, of a conjecturing and probing atmosphere become the norm in every classroom, not just in a few classes?

How might learners be encouraged to incorporate if…then… reasoning into their ordinary and mathematical language?

How can tasks be constructed which promote a sense of need to convince others (through unexpected or non-intuitive results, among others)?


Distinctions made included


Pragmatic and Formal-Academic reasoning;

Proof as Explanation and proof as Justification, which relates to the source of authority and warrants for conviction;

Empirical Induction (perhaps developing into exhaustion by cases); Deduction based on firm foundations; Abduction and Insight requiring further justification; Deduction based not on formal foundations but on accepted 'facts';

Must, May, and May-not, as possible states, related to notions of for-all and the nature of certainty and conviction;

Social reasoning and Mathematical reasoning (role of counter-example, empirical evidence, provision of warrants, etc.)


It was noted that in the domain of number and algebra, the transition from arithmetic thinking (working from the known towards the as-yet-unknown) to algebraic thinking (using the as-yet-unknown or the general to express structural relationships and then using symbol manipulation to find values for unknowns) is a significant hurdle for many learners. A popular strategy in the face of a problem is to use guess-and-test, which can be refined into trial-and-improvement, with the improvements made more and more systematically and with reference to structures in the problem, but there is still a gap between this and the use of algebraic language to express generality and-or structure. Spreadsheets offer promising support for making such a transition.


It was noted that whereas some countries are drawing back from specific attention to mathematical exploration and group work, others are embarking on emphasising these components of learners' experiences.


Some colleagues were asked to describe particularly the role and impact of technology with respect to reasoning and proof. Evidence was offered that spreadsheets provide a tool in which aspects of reasoning can be developed, especially in the transition between arithmetic and algebraic thinking through the use of cell-references as a form of symbolic expression and manipulation.


There was widespread agreement that we did not want to see curricula specified which encouraged teachers to get learners to memorise proofs, or otherwise mechanise the use of reasoning. It was noted that this does run counter to the force from teachers and learners for ever-more precisely specified problem types so that learners can succeed through familiarity with similar worked examples.


Although not discussed at length, many were concerned with equity issues, in that while access to mathematical reasoning must be provided for all future citizens, this does not mean that everyone develops at the pace of the slowest.


The meeting ended with participants discussing three questions chosen by the QCA representatives:


1. How can pedagogic structures such as

scientific-debate;
personal-work-group-work-leading-to-a-poster-collective-work-around-posters-synthesis-and-overview;
group-work-sharing-synthesising-extending-or-varying

and many variants become standard pedagogical structures in all classrooms?

2. The intended curriculum is attenuated by the taught curriculum which is in turn attenuated by the assessed curriculum. What forms might assessment take which focuses on reasoning without promoting rote memorisation and without promoting attempts to mechanise specific reasoning structures?

3. (Most urgent for UK). The Government will be instituting a programme for talented and gifted learners (at all ages) (say top 10%): what could be offered in place of simple acceleration, particularly in regard to reasoning?


Another way of thinking might be to ask

What could be sensibly and effectively implemented more or less immediately with suitable materials?

What could be sensibly and effectively implemented with suitable professional development for all mathematics teachers?

What could be sensibly and effectively implemented through curricular change and different assessment over the longer term?


If you want to see what was suggested, visit the QCA website (http://www.qca.org.uk) when the papers for and from the meeting are released.

© John Mason

 

Back to the Newsletter